I am a
Home I AM A Search Login

Which treatments for which patients?

RECENT POSTS

GLOBAL YEAR

The 2024 Global Year will examine what is known about sex and gender differences in pain perception and modulation and address sex-and gender-related disparities in both the research and treatment of pain.

Learn More >

How do clinicians decide which treatments to provide to which patients? One might assume a simple process: if patient X has condition Y, treatment Z is provided. For many conditions, it is this straightforward – treatment guidelines are clear enough to remove any guesswork. Unfortunately, clear guidelines do not exist for chronic pain. Even the most specific guidelines leave plenty of wiggle room; there are also conflicting recommendations across guidelines. This situation speaks to the complexity of chronic pain as highlighted by the biopsychosocial model. It also contributes to wide variability in chronic pain treatment.

My team is interested in explaining this variability. Along with others, we have documented the fact that some (much?) of that variability is due to factors that “should not” intrude on the decision-making process. Patient sex is one such factor that sometimes influences clinicians’ decision-making. Several studies have found that women with chronic pain receive less optimal care than men[1-5]. Documenting sex/gender disparities is an important first step. Unfortunately, the literature often stalls at this point – either by replicating these findings ad nauseum or by moving on to the next shiny object. Left behind is the question of “Why?” – Why are men and women with chronic pain treated differently?

To date, the small literature in this area has focused on patient factors. For example, perhaps men and women are treated differently because they have different preferences. These are important contributors, but they are not the only plausible explanations. What about clinician factors?

We know that interpersonal interactions are influenced by the sex/gender match of participants. Thus, women interact differently with women than with men, and men interact differently with men than with women. It seems reasonable, then, to speculate that clinician sex matters in the context of sex/gender disparities in pain care. We also know that sexist attitudes are prevalent in contemporary society, and that these attitudes affect a range of behaviours. There is no reason to think clinicians are immune to sexist attitudes – somehow inoculated by their clinical training. (Interestingly, some have argued that well-intentioned efforts to emphasize cultural competency in medical education actually perpetuate such attitudes (see Núñez[6])). Thus, clinicians’ sexist attitudes may also contribute to sex/gender disparities in pain care.

In a recent study[7], we examined how patient sex, clinician sex, and clinicians’ sexist attitudes influence pain treatment decisions. Using our established virtual human and lens model paradigm (see[8-11]), we asked 98 clinicians to make treatment (opioid, antidepressant, mental health referral, pain specialty referral) decisions for 16 patients – each vignette included a picture and text description of a patient with chronic low back pain. Subjects also completed a measure assessing their sexist attitudes.

We found 3 primary results:

  1. Female, but not male, subjects made different treatment decisions for male and female patients.
  2. Sex differences in treatment decisions were only found for “psychosocial” modalities (i.e., antidepressant and referral for counseling), with female patients receiving higher recommendation ratings than males.
  3. Male subjects endorsed more sexist attitudes than females, but these differences did not account for the sex differences in treatment decisions.

These results are consistent with the idea that pain in women is more likely to be “psychologised” than in men. But the story isn’t so simple: mood disorders are indeed more prevalent in women. Thus, are clinicians more likely to conclude that women’s pain is “in their head” and, thus, to treat with psychosocial interventions? Or are clinicians aware of base rate differences in psychiatric disorders between men and women and tailoring their treatments accordingly? We were not able to resolve this issue in our study, but it will be interesting to pursue in the future.

To thicken the pot further, only female clinicians showed these sex/gender disparities. What to make of this? There is some support in the general medical literature that female clinicians are more likely to consider a patient’s psychosocial situation when providing care[12-16]. Our study suggests this holds true for pain-specific care, with one potentially important caveat: the emphasis on psychosocial factors only occurs when treating female patients. To speculate, perhaps female clinicians are more aware of base rate differences in psychiatric disorders and/or more attentive to the psychological status of their female patients. Drawing from the literature on in-group favoritism[17], another possibility is that female clinicians err on the side of providing more care to female patients who are already vulnerable to undertreatment[1-5]. As we are so fond of saying, future research is needed…

What about clinicians’ sexist attitudes? Although previous studies suggest these attitudes influence care for male and female patients, ours was one of the first studies to test this hypothesis directly. Consistent with the general population, we found male clinicians scored higher on the sexism scale than female clinicians. This difference did not, however, account for the differential treatment recommendations for male and female patients. Curiously, only female clinicians made different treatment decisions for male and female patients, despite their lower sexism. Consistent with Nunez’s argument[6], more egalitarian female providers may aspire to provide the utmost in sex/gender-sensitive care. Although well-intentioned, this may lead to the inappropriate tailoring of pain treatment according to patient sex.

In summary, we found female (but not male) clinicians are more likely to recommend psychosocial treatments for female pain patients than for similar male patients. Clinicians’ sexist attitudes were not associated with these treatment differences. Further research is needed to flesh out the interaction of sex (patient and provider) and sexism in the context of pain care. Better understanding of the factors (patient, provider, contextual) that amplify and mitigate such differences may lead to targeted efforts to reduce sex/gender disparities and improve treatment for patients with pain.

About Adam Hirsh

Adam HirshAdam is a clinical health psychologist and Assistant Professor in the Department of Psychology at Indiana University – Purdue University Indianapolis. His lab conducts research on the biopsychosocial aspects of pain and functioning in humans. In one line of research, they use virtual human technology and mixed methodology to investigate pain judgments and clinical decision making. They are currently adapting this technology to test an intervention to enhance clinician empathy and improve clinical decision making. They are also currently conducting research on perceptions of injustice in children and families dealing with chronic pain, as well as research on racial/ethnic differences in pain expectations and coping. In addition to pain science, his laboratory is keenly interested in the wonders of craft beer and fine pastries.

References

  1. Cleeland C, Gonin R, Hatfield A, Edmonson J, Blum R, Stewart J, Pandya KJ: Pain and its treatment in outpatients with metastatic cancer. N Engl J Med 330:592-596, 1994
  2. Lord B, Cui J, Kelly A-M: The impact of patient sex on paramedic pain management in the prehospital setting. Am J Emerg Med 27:525-27:529, 2009
  3. McDonald DD: Gender and ethnic stereotyping and narcotic analgesic administration. Res Nurs Health 17: 45-49, 1994
  4. Michael GE, Sporer KA, Youngblood GM: Women are less likely than men to receive prehospital analgesia for isolated extremity injuries. Am J Emerg Med 25:901-906, 2007
  5. Safdar B, Heins A, Homel P, Miner J, Neighbor M, DeSandre P, Todd KH: Impact of physician and patient gender on pain management in the emergency department—A multicenter study. Pain Med 10:364-372, 2009
  6. Núñez AE: Transforming cultural competence into crosscultural efficacy in women’s health education. Acad Med 75:1071-1080, 2000
  7. Hirsh AT, Hollingshead NA, Matthias MS, Bair MJ, & Kroenke K (2014). The influence of patient sex, provider sex, and sexist attitudes on pain treatment decisions. J Pain PMID: 24576430
  8. Hirsh AT, Alqudah AF, Stutts LA, Robinson ME: Virtual human technology: Capturing sex, race, and age influences in individual pain decision policies. Pain 140:231-238, 2008
  9. Hirsh AT, George SZ, Robinson ME: Pain assessment and treatment disparities: A virtual human technology investigation. Pain 143:106-113, 2009
  10. Hirsh AT, Hollingshead NH, Bair MJ, Matthias MS, Wu J, Kroenke K: The influence of patient’s sex, race and depression on clinician pain treatment decisions. Eur J Pain 17:1569-1579, 2013
  11. Stutts L, Hirsh A, George S, Robinson M: Investigating patient characteristics on pain assessment using virtual human technology. Eur J Pain 14:1040-1045, 2010
  12. Bertakis KD, Franks P, Epstein RM: Patient-centered communication in primary care: Physician and patient gender and gender concordance. J Womens Health 18:539-545, 2009
  13. Lorber J, Moore LJ: Gender and the Social Construction of Illness, in Howard J, Risman B, Sprague J (eds)Vol. 4. Plymouth, UK, AltaMira Press, 2002
  14. Roter DL, Hall JA: Why physician gender matters in shaping the physician-patient relationship. J Womens Health 7:1093-1097, 1998
  15. Roter DL, Hall JA: Physician gender and patient centered communication: A critical review of empirical research. Annu Rev Public Health 25:497-519, 2004
  16. Roter DL, Hall JA, Aoki Y: Physician gender effects in medical communication. JAMA 288:756-764, 2002
  17. Fiske ST: What we know now about bias and intergroup conflict, the problem of the century. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11:123-128, 2002
Share this