I am a
Home I AM A Search Login

Phantom limb pain: peripheral or central origin?

RECENT POSTS

GLOBAL YEAR

The 2024 Global Year will examine what is known about sex and gender differences in pain perception and modulation and address sex-and gender-related disparities in both the research and treatment of pain.

Learn More >

In a recent paper by Vaso et al. (2014) it was suggested that phantom limb pain is driven primarily by activity generated within the ectopic dorsal root ganglia (DRG) (they wrote for BiM on it here). Ectopic activity is abnormal spontaneous activity generated in neuromas in the residual limb as well as in dorsal root ganglions. The authors reported that a peripheral nerve block yielded total or major relief from phantom limb pain in 31 consecutive amputees of this study. They conclude that ectopic afferent discharge is necessary for post-amputation phantom limb pain and undermines the current dogma that phantom limb pain originates centrally.

Contrary, neuroscientific investigation of mechanisms behind phantom limb pain suggests changes in both the central and peripheral nervous system. Functional adaptations to the amputation in the central nervous system have been connected to phantom limb pain (Flor et al. 1995; Harris 1999), while at the same time both residual limb pain and phantom limb pain have been associated with altered peripheral nociceptive input originating from the residual limb or the dorsal root ganglion (Devor 2013). Indeed, bottom-up and top-down processes could be complementary, as described previously (Flor et al. 2006).

Vaso et al. (2014) suggests that ectopic activity in stump neuromas and the DRG might lead to an increased cortical representation of the (phantom) limb, as recently reported (Makin et al. 2013), and that this proportional increase is evidence for a bottom-up process in phantom limb pain. But it is important to note that the Makin et al study looked at SI activation during phantom movement/motor imagery, whereas Flor’s early work looked at SI activation during the tactile stimulation of the body part adjacent to the representation of the amputated limb in the topographic SI map (the lip in arm amputees). This is made all the more complex by a recent study that used a physiologically realistic, computational model to help work it out (Boström et al. 2014). They reported that both the amount of brain reorganization, and the level of cortical activity during phantom movement, was enhanced in a (computer-based) scenario with ‘strong phantom pain’, as compared to one with ‘weak phantom pain’. So, what does it all mean? In the commentary (Foell et al. 2014) we wrote for PAIN, we suggested that phantom pain, maladaptive reorganization, and persistent representation during phantom movements, may all be caused by the same underlying mechanism. One candidate, as suggested by Vaso et al, is abnormally increased spontaneous activity of denervated nociceptive channels. That is, the loss of adequate sensory and nociceptor input in combination with subsequent maladaptive reorganization in the injured nerves (especially within C-fibers) leads to increased noisy activity in afferent pathways. This idea shares some similarities with current theories of tinnitus – the frequency of the noise matches a notch of hearing loss – it is as though the nervous system ‘fills in’ the missing data and this leads to ongoing noise at this frequency.

Additionally, continuing spontaneous peripheral input has been demonstrated in amputees without phantom pain (Mackert et al. 2003), and a model reconciling cortical and peripheral mechanisms has been proposed previously (Spitzer et al. 1995). There is one outstanding consideration here, however: every amputee loses peripheral input, but only some get phantom limb pain. So what is different? This up-regulated system or the fact that it causes pain? More research is required!

About Martin Diers

Martin DiersMartin Diers, PhD, is a psychologist in the Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience at the Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, in Mannheim, Germany. He was trained in neuroscientific pain research and has an additional clinical training in behavioral treatment. Any questions about his work can be directed to him at martin.diers@zi-mannheim.de

Reference

Boström KJ, de Lussanet MH, Weiss T, Puta C, & Wagner H (2014). A computational model unifies apparently contradictory findings concerning phantom pain. Scientific reports, 4 PMID: 24931344

Devor M. Neuropathic pain: pathophysiological response of nerves to injury. In: McMahon SL, Koltzenburg M, editors. Wall and Melzack’s textbook of pain. London: Churchill Livingstone; 2013. p. 861–88.

Flor H, Elbert T, Knecht S, Wienbruch C, Pantev C, Birbaumer N, Larbig W, & Taub E (1995). Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature, 375 (6531), 482-4 PMID: 7777055

Flor H, Nikolajsen L, Staehelin Jensen T. Phantom limb pain: a case of maladaptive CNS plasticity? Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7:873–81.

Foell J, Andoh J, Bekrater-Bodmann R, Diers M, Fuchs X, Colloca L, & Flor H (2014). Peripheral origin of phantom limb pain: is it all resolved? Pain, 155 (10), 2205-6 PMID: 25180014

Harris, A. (1999). Cortical origin of pathological pain The Lancet, 354 (9188), 1464-1466 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)05003-5

Mackert BM, Sappok T, Grüsser S, Flor H, & Curio G (2003). The eloquence of silent cortex: analysis of afferent input to deafferented cortex in arm amputees. Neuroreport, 14 (3), 409-12 PMID: 12634493

Makin TR, Scholz J, Filippini N, Henderson Slater D, Tracey I, Johansen-Berg H. Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nat Commun 2013;4:1570.

Spitzer M, Böhler P, Weisbrod M, Kischka U. A neural network model of phantom limbs. Biol Cybern 1995;72:197–206.

Vaso A, Adahan HM, Gjika A, Zahaj S, Zhurda T, Vyshka G, & Devor M (2014). Peripheral nervous system origin of phantom limb pain. Pain, 155 (7), 1384-91 PMID: 24769187

Share this